суббота, 18 мая 2019 г.
Acculturation, Biculturism and Marginalization Essay
Ross-Sheriff (2011) commented that outside(a) migration patterns dupe * changed as a consequence of broad kindly, political, economical, and environmental * trends and explained the causes of the driving forces were including war, * globalization, urbanization, and changing ethnical norms regarding kind roles and * responsibilities (Ross-Sheriff, 2011).With these complex trends of migration * patterns, wagon train Hear (2010) viewed migration as a march which was an integral part * of broader social transformations, but which too had its birth internal kinetics with * other pointors related to the migrating process, shaping social transformation in their * own way. Migration was too linked in complex ways to class, gender, generation, * hea whenceishity and other social factors, which were embodied in positions in fireside and host * communities, and in work and domestic relationships, yet of which efficiency be * transformed in the operate of the migratory proc ess (Van Hear, 2010).To pull in this complex process of migration, especially under changing circumstances of peerless culture to a nonher, it might be useful to build conceptual tools for understanding these transitory processes in migration studies and in social science much widely (Van hear, 2010). They also include mediating agents and transitions that pauperism also to be accounted for, as well as intersections among class, gender, generation, ethnicity and other social ruptures as well as the main driving forces of migration (Van Hear, 2010).Of hightail it thither were other all important(p) concepts such as relations in the midst of age and space, between dynamics or processes and outcomes, and between structure and agency that needed to get attention (Van Hear, 2010). However, it is impossible to discuss all disaccordent theoretical concepts tangled in several(predicate) types of migration process in the current limited film.Rather, this scan tried to focus on p sychological impacts such as ethnic one-on-oneity and self-esteem on migration through socializing processes particularly on family- related migration because contrary patterns of migration produced different communities and resulted in producing different unsettled identities including varying levels of psychological di underscore (J stars, 2008). Further, few falsifiable studies have focused on migrant adults cosmoss. closely migrants identification related literatures tended to relate more for adolescents or young children because identity formation might be particularly challenging in this cohort, especially when the values and beliefs of their natal culture differed significantly from those of the host hunting lodge (Sodowsky, Kwan, & Pannu, 1995 as cited in Farver, Narang, & Bhadha, 2002). Therefore, this study focused on ethnic identity and self-identification issues of adult migrants themselves inwardly a family structure according to different theoretical models re levant to adaptation of new cultures, because family was the basic instrument in the society (Nesdale, Rooney, & Smith, 1997).In fact, almost ethnical acquisition theories developed and evolved in 1990s when international migration became a primal issue in international politics at the beginning of 1990s. As Castle (2002) argued that migration, reading and international relations were closely connected as migration was a major factor of transformation for both(prenominal) sending and receiving countries for different types of migrants (Castle, 2002). With this perspective, this study generally focused on those migration culture acquisition theories developed in 1990 rather then looking at current perspectives in the most recent literatures, which actually have evolved from these original theories in 1990s (Castle, 2002).As the findings from these explore studies has had been mixed or sometimes contradictory, it was important to understand the exact nature of the relationship b etween migrant ethnic identification and the enculturation process both need to be specified and assessed properly with coherent measurements and theoretical premises (Nesdale et al. , 1997). Important theoretical concepts ethnic identity, cultivation, biculturism, and marginalisation. consort to Phinney (1990 as cited in Farver, Narang & Bhadha., 2002), ethnic identity and polish were related but separate constructs.Ethnic identity involves an individuals self-identification as a group member, a sense of belonging to an ethnic group, attitudes toward ethnic group of membership, and degree of ethnic group involvement (Farver et al. , 2002). The term culture was defined in anthropology as those phenomena, which resulted when groups of individuals having different cultures came into continuous first-hand contact with subsequent changes in the original pattern of either or both groups (Redfield, Linton, & Herskovits, 1936 as cited in Birman, 1994).Although acculturation was a neu tral term in this context (that is, change might take place in either or both groups), in practice, acculturation tended to induce more changes in one of the groups than in the other ( cull, 1990a as cited in berry, 1997) Berry (1997) argued that in all plural form societies, cultural groups and their individual members, in both dominant and non-dominant situations, must deal with the issue of how to acculturate.According to Berry (1997), four acculturation strategies were introduced assimilation, separation, marginalisation, and integration. When individuals do not wish to maintain their cultural identity and seek free-and-easy interaction with other new cultures, the assimilation strategy is defined. In contrast, when individuals place a value on holding on to their original culture, and at the equal time wish to avoid interaction with others, then the separation is defined (Berry, 1997).When there is an interest in both maintaining ones original culture, magical spell in dail y interactions with other groups, integration is the option here, there is some degree of cultural equity maintained, while at the same time seeking to participate as an integral part of the bigger social network (Berry, 1997). Last, when there is little possibility or interest in cultural living (often for reasons of enforced cultural loss), and little interest in having relations with others (often for reasons of exclusion or discrimination) then marginalization is defined (Berry, 1997).However, this acculturation categories model has been criticized methodologically (Rudmin, 2003, 2009 as cited in Schwartz et al. , 2010) because all four of Berrys categories were represented in the same way by creating the two by two matrix of acculturation categories between high and low. However, the cut off point between high and low was arbitrary and would differ across samples, making comparisons across studies difficult, resulting in the fact that all four categories existed and were equa lly valid (Rudmin, 2003 as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010) and suggesting that not all of Berrys categories might exist in a attached sample or population, and that some categories might have multiple subtypes (Schwartz et al. , 2010).In particular, Berry (1997) viewed the term biculturism as referring to acculturation that involved the individual simultaneously in the two cultures that were in contact in endogenetic ways, which appeared to be a consistent predictor of more positive outcomes than the three alter immanents of assimilation, separation, or marginalization.Berry and his colleagues (Sam & Berry, 1995) assessed the acculturation strategies of various immigrant groups in northwest America and the results showed that bicultural individuals undergo less acculturative stress, concern and fewer psychological problems significantly, while marginalized individuals suffered the most psychological distress, including problems with self-identification and cultural alienation, which adversely affected their self-esteem (Farver et al. , 2002).However, Shiraev and Levy (2007) explained acculturative stress as a negative ruleing that a marginalized mortal might experience as a distressing psychological reaction to any unfamiliar cultural environment based on the premiss that person and groups undergoing any social and cultural change should experience a certain amount of psychological distress. Generally, many early definitions of acculturation focused on exposure to two cultures simultaneously as a culture shock, which was a reactive state of specific pathology or deficit, rather than taking advantage of universe bicultural (Berry & Annis, 1974 Shiraev et al., 2007).The validity of marginalization as an come on to acculturation by Berry (1997) was also questioned (Del Pilar & Udasco, 2004 as cited in Schwartz et al. , 2010). Schawartz et al. argued that the likelihood that a person would develop a cultural sense of self without picture on either the h eritage or receiving cultural contexts would be less likely to. The marginalization approach might be true only for the small segment of migrants who rejected both their heritage and receiving cultures (Berry, 2006b).Indeed, studies exploitation empirically based clustering methods have raise small or nonexistent marginalization groups and scales that assay to measure marginalization typically had poor reliability and validity compared with scales for the other categories (Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995 Unger et al. , 2002 as cited in Schwartz et al. , 2010). As described earlier, the impact of migrant ethnic identity on psychological distress had proportionally versatile points of views if they were either negative or positive reactions, depending on different theoretical frames.For example, Social indistinguishability possibility (Tajfel & Turner, 2001) and Self-Categorization Theory (Turner, 1987) accentuate more on the importance to individuals of their identification with particular social groups. Social Identity Theory (Tajfel & Turner, 2001 as cited in Yip, Gee, & Takeuchi, 2008) viewed a possible explanation for why ethnic identity might buffer the effects of discrimination. According to this theory,individuals chose from an array of possible social identity groups and, once those groups were chosen, individuals focused on the positive flavors of their in-group, which helped to boost their own esteem, suggesting that ethnic identity was more important to their overall identity (Yip et al. , 2009).In contrast, if ethnicity was a central component of ones identity, it might actually exacerbate the effects of discrimination, resulting in a greater negative impact on mental health, according to self-categorization theory (Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987 as cited in Yip et al., 2008), suggesting that people should be more in tune with environmental cues that were relevant to an important aspect of their identity.That is, experien ces of racial discrimination might be such a cue relevant to their ethnic identity. Indeed, investigate suggested that African American adults and adolescents who reported strong racial centrality were also more likely to report experiences of racial discrimination (Neblett, Shelton, & Sellers, 2004 Sellers, Caldwell, Schmeelk-Cone, & Zimmerman, 2003 Sellers & Shelton, 2003 as cited in Yip et al., 2008).However, despite this emphasis by social theorists, they tended to forget the larger literature that involved with both ethnicity and the acculturation process (Liebkind, 1993 1996 as cited in Nesdale, Rooney & Smith, 1997). setoff of all, these different findings resulted from wishing of inclusion of acculturation itself as a variable methodologically when acculturation was considered as a phenomenon in seek designs (Sam and Berry, 2006).Without including acculturation as a variable, the explanations for human behavior similarities and differences across populations would remai n incomplete (Sam et al. , 2006). Second, a further criticism of the acculturation literatures was that the same two acculturation processes, and the same four-acculturation categories, characterized all migrants equally disregarding of the type of migrant, the countries of origin and settlement, and the ethnic group in question, according to Berrys (1980) model and other similar approaches (Sam et al., 2006).Finally, the vast majority of studies in the acculturation literature have focused on behavioural acculturation (Schwartz et al. , 2010). That is, most widely used acculturation measures included primarily (or only) items assessing language use and other cultural practices (e. g. , Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995 Stephenson, 2000 Szapocznik, Kurtines, & Fernandez, 1980 as cited in Schwartz et al. , 2010) imputable to accepting the fact that cultural practices might provide only a fair proxy for cultural adaptation (Schwartz et al., 2010).Theoretical frameworks for acculturat ion research Shiraev & Levy (2007) claimed that cross-cultural psychologists usually used three approaches to examine human activities in various cultural settings. They were the sociobiological approach, the sociological approach and eco-cultural approach (Shiraev et al. , 2007). In particular, the eco-cultural approach emphasized both the environment and the individual were seen as promiscuous and interchanging systems (Shiraev et al., 2007), introducing John Berry whom originally developed this theory further in contemporary cross-cultural psychology.Shiraev et al. (2007) also pointed out that specialists should to be able to explain how, why, and to what extent people differed from one another, when ecological, biological, cultural, and acculturation factors were identify and interpreted into consideration (Berry, J. W. , Poortinga, Y. H. , Segall, M. H. , & Dasen. P. R. ,1992 as cited in Shiraev et al. , 2007). In related to the concerns pointed by Shiraev et al.(2007), Berry (1997) argued earlier there were important links between cultural context and individual behavioural development, demonstrating what happened to individuals who developed in one cultural context when attempting to re-establish their lives in another one through his acculturation research framework, by confirming the fact that acculturation was one of the most complex areas of research in cross-cultural psychology because the process involved more than one culture and in two distinct senses (Berry, 1997).According to Berry (1997), the concept of acculturation was use to refer to the cultural changes resulting from different ethnic groups encountered, while the concepts of psychological acculturation and adaptation were employed to refer to the psychological changes and eventual outcomes that occur as a result of individuals experiencing acculturation.In another words, acculturation phenomena resulted from contact between two or more cultures and research on acculturation had to be c omparative in order to understand variations in psychological outcomes that were the result of cultural variations in the two groups in contact (Berry, 1997).In particular, this framework viewed the integration model of acculturation strategies the most desirable among other strategies, considering it the same as the biculturalism model (Berry, 1997). For example, Berry and his colleagues (Berry, 1980 Berry, J. W. , Kim, U. , Power, S. , Young, M, & Bujaki, M. , 1989 Berry, Kim, Minde, & Mok, 1987Sam & Berry, 1995 as cited in Farver et al., 2002) assessed the acculturation strategies of various immigrant groups in North America and the result showed that integration was the most psychologically adaptive attitude, arguing that integrated or bicultural individuals experienced less acculturative stress and anxiety and manifested fewer psychological problems than those who were marginalized, separated, or assimilated, whereas marginalized individuals suffered the most psychological dist ress, including problems with self-identification and cultural alienation, which also affected their self-esteem (Farver et al., 2002). However, Phinney, Cantu, and Kurtz (1997) found that American identity was associated with self-esteem only for non-Hispanic whiteneds, but not for other ethnic groups. These mixed results as explained above raised two issues in the acculturation literatures. First of all, cultural practices might offer only a substitute for cultural adpatations, as Portes and Rumbaut (2001 as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010) mentioned that many Asiatic American young adults in their sample were not proficient in their native languages, even though they still sensed their identification with their parents countries of origin and maintained many of their values (Schwarz et al. , 2010). Secondly, most researchers on biculturism did not sufficiently define an accurate operational definition of biculturism so that interpretation of those research results were problema tic (Birman, 1994).Indeed, one finding in the United States, was that self-identification as American was markedly higher in non-Hispanic Whites than in ethnic minority groups (e. g. , Devos & Banaji, 2005 as cited in Schwartz et al. , 2010) and many White Americans did not perceived themselves as members of an ethnic group (Schildkraut, 2007 as cited in Schwartz et al. , 2010). In brief, different operational definition problems of acculturation arose from different theoretical models of acculturation regarding to their assumptions (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993).LaFromboise et al. (1993) imitation acculturation as one of substitutes among the biculturism models. Biculturism as defined in this theory was viewed as the alternation model, which implied an individual in two culture contacts could be competent in both cultures without losing one of the cultures competencies in distinct cultural contexts as alternation model, whereas, fusion model meant a blended cultural identi ty, consisting of a synthesis of aspects of both cultures (LaFromboise et al., 1993).However, Berrys (1997) integrating approach of biculturism differed from the bicultural model (LaFromboise et al. , 1993 as cited in Birman, 1994) and it emphasized more on the relationship between the two cultural groups based on its implicit assumption that one of two cultures were higher than the other inwardly a virtuoso social structure (LaFromboise et al. , 1993).Benet-Martinez and colleagues found that blended bicultural individuals tended to report higher self-esteem and lower psychological distress than a marginal population (Chen et al. , 2008 as cited in Schwartz et al. , 2010) because the consistent availability of both cultural flows within the persons normal life increased the ease of activating the correct cultural schema in accordance with their environmental situations (Schwartz et al. , 2010).In contrast, Tadmor, Tetlock, and Peng (2009) argued that the bicultural model consider ed those marginal individuals in positive ways, when there was little interest in cultural maintenance and little interest in having relations with others, suggesting positive aspects of being a marginal person might be (1) sharing his or her condition with others of the same original culture (2) engaging in institutional practices that were shared by other marginal people (3) experiencing no major frustration from social expectations and (4) still perceiving himself or herself to be a member of a group (LaFromboise et al., 1993).According to Sam and Berry (2006), many studies of how migrants coped with intercultural contacts had discrepancies in the ways in which they were operationalized and measured. As no standardized or widely evaluate acculturation measures existed, it was necessary to design a clear and explicit formulation of acculturation instrument in order to assess acculturation adequately (Sam et al. , 2006).Further Sam and Berry (2006) pointed out that most empirical studies widely used a self-report type of questionnaires that had been recognized limitations such as social desirability, emphasizing obtaining divergent validation by source of information other than the respondents reports. Therefore, it is vital to understand each theory within its specific assumptions and not to generalize across all situations regardless of their similar findings (LaFromboise et al. , 1993).As this study detect migrants acculturation processes so far within specific theoretical frameworks, literature findings in different research were mixed as to whether individuals could be highly acculturated and at the same time be strongly identified with their ethnic group (Farver, Narang, & Bhadha. , 2002). These confusing problems initially evolved because of the context in which migration arrangements and their acculturation processes were basically transformed and more and more uncertain due to globalization (Landolt & Da, 2005).Shiraev & Levy (2007) suggested a new approach to cross-cultural psychology in the twenty-first century, which was linked to the concept of globalization. planetaryization was defined as a proliferation of cross-border flow and transnational networks due to new technologies of communication and transport that allowed frequent and multi-directional streams of people, ideas and cultural symbols (Castle, 2010). Castle also argued that globalization leads to major changes in the character of international migration. In other words, the context for migrant incorporation has already changed radically and allow continue to do so.The rise of multiculturalism itself rather than assimilation or biculturism is one sign of this, but is not the end of the story new forms of identity and belonging go beyond multiculturalism (Castle, 2010). Even though there is limited empirical evidence for clear statements for globalization, there probably are highly cosmopolitan groups who feel at home everywhere such as global business and p rofessional elites might equate with this image. But most members of transnational communities fall between these extremes, and probably have contradictory and fluctuating identities (Castle, 2002).Conclusions This study explored that a special case of cultural psychology was the study of how individuals respond to situations where they were in transition between their original culture and another that differed from it in some respects in terms of acculturation, especially within a specific theoretical frame that could apply to the specific situation (Adler & Gielen, 1994).There was no single theory widely accepted by all social scientists to agree with the emergence and perpetuation of international migration patterns in the world under globalization (Van Hear, 2010),suggesting that the contemporary migrating context in which such migrating arrangements were realized fundamentally kept transforming so that it became increasingly uncertain (Landolt and Da, 2005) Although the topic o f cultural contact and individual change has attracted right smart attention in contemporary cross-cultural psychology, the field has been characterized by a lack of theoretical coherence, definitional problems with key constructs, and single sample studies that limit the external validity of empirical cross-cultural research (Ward and Kenney, 1994).As acculturation is a process which takes place over time, and which results in changes both in the culture and in the individual culture changes, it would be ideal o compare two sets of data are compared over time using the same people. However, in practice, it is impossible in most acculturation research settings (Sam et al. , 2006). Instead, a parkland alternative to longitudinal research is cross-sectional research in which a time-related variable, such as duration of residence or generational status can be used for the generalizability of acculturation theories (Sam et al., 2006).In general, researchers of migrating studies need to be aware that it is the selective nature of the sample that happens across all migrating research. That is, individuals who chose to migrate would be different from those who do not (Sodowsky, G. , Kwan, K. , & Pannu, R. , 1995 as cited in Farver et al. , 1997). Finally, acculturation research generally focused on immigrants assumed to be permanently settled in their new host countries. As a result, the terms migrants or international migrants referred to the same type of migrants collectively.Moreover, many countries were both sending and receiving countries for different types of migrants, or in the process of transition from one type to the other (Castel, 2002). Therefore, where applicable, it is viable to design acculturation research studies classifying different types of migrants. References Adler, L. L. , & Gielen, U. P. (Eds. ). (1994). cross-cultural topics in psychology. Westport Praeger Publishers. Berry, J. W. (1980). Social and cultural change. In Triandis, H. C. , & B rislin, R. (Eds. ). vade mecum of cross-cultural psychology (pp. 211-279). Boston Allyn & Bacon. Berry, J.W. , Kim, U. , Power, S. , Young, M, & Bujaki, M. (1989). Acculturation attitudes in plural societies. Applied Psychology An International Review, 38, 185-206 Berry, (1990a). Psychology of acculturation. In Berman, J. (Eds. ). Cross-cultural perspectives Nebraska Symposium on need (pp. 201-234). Lincoln University of Nebraska Press. Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaption. Applied Psychology An international review, 46(1), 5-68. Berry, J. W. , & Annis, R. (1974).Acculturation stress. diary of cross-cultural Psychology, 5(4), 382-397. Berry, J. W. , Kim, U., Minde, T. , & Mok, D. (1987). Comparative studies of acculturative stress. International Migration Review, 21, 591-511. Berry, J. W. , Poortinga, Y. H. , Segall, M. H. , & Dasen. P. R. (1992). Cross-cultural Psychology Research and application. New York Cambridge university Press. Birman, D. (1994). A cculturation and human diversity in a multicultural society. In Trickett, E. J. , Watts, R. J. , & Birman D, (Eds. ). Perspectives on people in context (pp. 261-284). San Franscisco Jossey-Bass Inc. Castele, S. (2002). Migration and community formation under conditions of globalization.The summation for Migration Studies of New York, 36(4), 1143- 1168. Cuellar, I. , Arnold, B. , & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans-II a revision of the original ARSMA scale. Hispanic ledger of Behavioral Science, 17, 275-304. inside 10. 1177/07399863950173001 DelPilar, J. A. , & Udasco, J. O. (2004). Deculturation Its lack of validity. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 10, 169-176. inside 10. 1037/1099- 9809. 10. 2. 169 Devos, T. , & Banaji, M. R. (2005). American = White? daybook of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 447-466. doi 10. 1037/0022-3514.88. 3. 447 Farver, J. A. , Narang, S. K. , & Bhadha, B. R. (2002). East meets west Ethnic ide ntity, acculturation, and conflict in Asian Indian families. Journal of Family Psychology, 16(3), 338-350. doi 10. 1037//0893-3200. 16. 3. 338 Jones, A. (2008). A silent but mighty river the costs of womens economic migration. Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 33(4), 761-807. Landolt, D. , & Da, W. W. (2005). The Spatially Ruptured Practices of Migrant Families A Comparison of Immigrants from El Salvador and the Peoples Republic of China. Current Sociology, 53, 625-652. doi 10. 1177/0011392105052719.LaFromboise. , T. , Coleman. , H. , & Gerton (1993). Psychological impact of biculturism Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 394-412. Liebkind, K. (1993). Self-reported ethnic identity, depression and anxiety among youth Vietnamese refugees and their parents. Journal of Refugee Studies, 6, 25-39. Neblett, E, Shelton, J. N. , & Sellers, R. M. (2004). The role of racial identity in managing daily racial hassles. In Philogene, G. (Eds. ). Race and identity The legacy of Kenneth Clark. Washington DC American Psychological Association Press. Nesdale. , D. , Rooney. , R. , & Smith. , L.(1997). Migrant ethnic identity and psychological distress. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28(5), 569-588. doi 10. 1177/0022022197285004 Phinney, J. S. (1990). When we talk about American ethic groups, what do we mean? American Psychologist, 51, 918-917. Phinney, J. S. , & Ong, A. D. (2007). Conceptualization and measurement of ethnic identity Current status and future directions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 271-281. doi 10. 1037/0022-0167. 54. 3. 271 Portes, A. , & Rumbaut, R. G. (2001). Legacies The story of the immigrant second generation. Berkerly University of California Press.Redfield, R. , Linton, R. , & Herskovits, M. J. (1936) memorandum on the study of acculturation. American Anthropologist, 38, 149-152. Ross-Sheriff, F. (2011). Global migration and gender. Journal of Women and Social Works, 26(3), 233-238a. doi 10. 1177/0886109911417692 R udmin, F. W. (2003). overcritical history of the acculturation psychology of assimilation, separation, integration, and marginalization. Review of General Psychology, 7, 3-37. doi 10. 1177/01461670731197 Sam, D. , & Berry, J. W. (1995). Acculturative stress among young immigrants in Norway. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 36, 10-24.Sam, D. , & Berry, J. W. (2006). The Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology Electronic version. Retrieved from http//www. qut. eblib. com. au. ezp01. library. qut. edu. au/patron. Schildkraut, D. J. (2007). Defining American identity in the 21st century How much there is there? Journal of Politics, 69, 597-615. doi 10. 1111/j. 1468-2508. 2007. 00562. x Schwartz, S. J. , Unger, J. B. , Zamboanga, B. L. , & Szapocznik, J. (2010). Rethinking the concept of acculturation Implications for the theory and research. American Psychologist, 65(4), 237-251. doi 10. 1037/a0019330 Sellers, R.M. , Caldwell, C. H. , Schmeelk-Cone, K. H. & Zimmerman, M. A. ( 2003). racial identity, racial discrimination, perceived stress, and psychological distress among African American young adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 44(3), 302-317. Seller, R. M. , & Shelton, R. M. (2000). The role of racial identity in perceived racial discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(5), 1079-1092. Shiraev, E. , & Levy, D. (2007). Cross-Cultural Psychology Critical thinking and contemporary applications. Boston Pearson Education Inc. Sodowsky, G. , Kwan, K. , & Pannu, R., (1995). Ethnic identity of Asians in the United States.In J. Ponterotto (Ed. ), Handbook of multicultural counseling (pp. 110- 130). Newbury Park Sage. Stephenson, M. (2000). Development and validation of the Stephenson Multigroup Acculturation Scale (SMAS). Psychological Assessment, 12, 77-88. doi 10. 1037/1040-3590. 12. 1. 77 Szapocznik, J. , Kurtines, W. , & Fernandez, T. (1980). Bicultural involvement and adjustment in Hispanic-American youths. International Journal of Interculture Relations, 4, 353-365. Tadmor, C. T. , Tedlock, P. E. , & Peng, K. (2009).Acculturation strategies and integrative complexity The congnitive implications of biculturism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 105-139. doi 10. 1177/0022022108326279 Tajfel, H. , & Turner, J. C. (2001). An Integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Hogg, M. , & Abrams, D. (Eds. ). The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 94-109). New York Psychology Press. Turner, J. C. , Hogg, M. A. , Oakes, P. J. , Reicher, S. D. & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group A self-categorization theory. Oxford Balckwell. Unger, J. B. , Gallagher, P. , Shakib, S. , Ritt-Olson, A. , Palmer, P. H. , & Johnson, C.A. (2002). The AHIMSA acculturation scale A new measure of acculturation for adolescents in a multicultural society. Journal of Early Adolescence, 22, 225-251. doi 10. 1177/02731602022003001 Van Hear, N. (2010). Theories of migration and social change. Journal of Ethic and Migration Studies, 36(10), 1531-1536. doi 10. 1080/1369183X. 2010. 489359 Yip, T. , G, C. G. , & Takeuchi, D. T. (2008). Racial discrimination and psychological distress The impact of ethnic identity and age among immigrant and United States-born Asian adult. Dev Psychol, 44(3), 787-800. doi 10. 1037/0012-1649. 44. 3. 787.
Подписаться на:
Комментарии к сообщению (Atom)
Комментариев нет:
Отправить комментарий
Примечание. Отправлять комментарии могут только участники этого блога.